I also don't intend to spend money to read the Georgia study.
The Texas study uses a definition of "castle doctrine" that's a bit different from what we're used to: they use "expanded castle doctrine" to refer to what we'd call "stand your ground" laws. (See table 1, p. 36, for a summary of laws in the states they studied.)
Be that as it may, a couple of posts seem to reflect a basic misunderstanding of the methodology of the Texas study. The authors are using results
across states to compare expected and actual changes in crime rates, with adoption of castle doctrine laws as the independent variable. From the introduction:
. . .we primarily identify effects by comparing changes in castle doctrine states to other states in the same region of the country by including region-by-year fixed effects. Thus, the crucial identifying assumption is that in the absence of the castle doctrine laws, adopting states would have experienced changes in crime similar to non-adopting states in the same region of the country.
The graphs in Figure 1 make this comparison directly for experimental and control states. The data in Figure 2 are before-and-after (adoption of "castle doctrine" laws) comparisons within states, but they're comparing the
differences from the control (non-castle-doctrine) states. They show consistent increases in those differences after the adoption of the new laws.
It misses the point to critique the study on the basis of changes in crime rates within a particular state. The fact that one doesn't understand the methodology doesn't invalidate it.
As to robbery and burglary, it would be odd if they were excluded, given that muggings and other armed robberies, as well as most break-ins, are committed with the intent of stealing rather than committing mayhem on the victims; robbery and burglary rates are obvious dependent variables in a study of whether these laws deter crime.